test
Ted Cruz’s Neanderthal Brain
Rick Shenkman
Rick Shenkman is the editor of
HNN and the author of Political Animals: How Our Stone-Age Brain Gets in the Way
of Smart Politics (Basic Books, January 2016).
This article includes a brief excerpt from the book.
In March 1951, nine months
into the Korean War, Freda Kirchwey, a crusading liberal journalist at the Nation, expressed
bewilderment at Americans indifference to the fate of Korean civilians
killed by our bombs. The destruction was awful.
Nothing, she complained in a column, “excuses the terrible shambles
created up and down the Korean peninsula by the American-led forces, by
American planes raining down napalm and fire bombs, and by heavy land and naval
artillery.” And yet few seemed bothered by it.
Because she was an
optimist Kirchwey expressed the hope that Americans would eventually come to
share her own moral anguish at what was being done in their name. But they never did. If anything, Americans grew less interested
in the fate of the victims of our bombing the longer the war ground on.
Why didn’t Americans show
empathy for the victims? Social
scientists say it’s because empathy is actually very hard for human beings
operating in a modern setting. It’s hard
because in many circumstances an empathic response is an unnatural act. It is
not natural for humans to feel empathy for people who look different and speak
a different language. It is not natural
for us to feel empathy for those who are invisible to us as the bombing victims
were. Nor is it natural for us to feel
empathy for people who are of low status as were the Korean peasants we were
killing. Recent studies show
that when we are faced with a choice of killing a single individual to save the
lives of several we are far more apt to do it if the individual we are
sacrificing is of low status. When subjects in an experiment are told that the
people being saved are of high status the number of people willing to let the
low status victim die increases.
Another social science
finding helps us understand why empathy is often in short supply and why Ted
Cruz is capable of cavalierly recommending we carpet bomb Syrians living under
ISIS control. Once we have decided on a
course of action and convinced ourselves that it’s correct – as Americans
became convinced during the Korean War that we had to bomb the hell out of
Korea in order to stave off a communist victory and as we are now convinced our
war against ISIS is right and just – our brain helps us overcome any hint of
guilt we may be inclined to feel over the loss of life by dehumanizing the
victim. This is a classic
case of cognitive dissonance.
Our brain hates to feel torn between conflicting emotions. So it rationalizes doing what it wants to do
by discounting the feeling giving rise to negative emotion, in this case,
guilt. An extreme example is what
happened when the Nazis decided to sideline Jews and
later wipe them out. From the moment the Nazis began their ruthless
anti-Semitic campaigns they used hideous images to convince people that Jews
were little different than rats. It’s
far easier to kill someone if you can convince yourself they aren’t really
human. Rather than feeling empathy for the downtrodden Jews Nazis felt contempt
and disgust. These strong emotions
swamped whatever other feelings they might have felt deep down. In a study a few
years ago researchers measured the activity in the brains of subjects looking
at pictures of homeless people. The
finding was shocking. Brain activity in
the medial prefrontal cortex, the region of the brain where empathy is often
registered, was significantly lower. In
other words, we literally pay them no mind (or less mind).
This sounds cruel and
uncaring. But as far as biology is
concerned it makes perfect sense. Our genes
are selfish, as biologist Richard Dawkins teaches. That means they are built to enhance their
replication. Replication, in effect, is
their biological imperative. Caring for
people who are low in status, particularly those who belong to another tribe, doesn’t
serve this imperative. Indeed, it interferes with it by diverting the attention
of the host – that’s you and me – from activities that will enhance survival.
We don’t have to make a
conscious decision to ignore the fate of people who are low in status. Our brain does this automatically and
seamlessly. Out of conscious awareness
it decides if someone is useful to us.
If the person is, our brain suddenly achieves a state of hyper
attentiveness: our nostrils flare, the
eyes widen, the ears tune in relevant sounds. Think of
what happens to you when you’re in the presence of somebody important like
Bill Gates and you’ll know what I mean. And
if someone is deemed useless to us?
Unless we are worried that they pose a threat, our brain tells our body
to relax.
Because it is in our
biological interest to feel empathy for people from our own tribe and family –
people in a position to either enhance our survival or perpetuate our genes –
we come equipped with mechanisms to help us distinguish our people from
outsiders. From the moment we’re born we
focus on the people around us and bond with them. A mother and child know each other through
smell. Brother and sister recognize in
each other’s familiar facial features. When
we hear someone speaking a foreign language we instinctively discount their
humanity. This was shown in a 2014 experiment designed to determine if
human beings are more willing to sacrifice someone who speaks a different
language in order to save the lives of several others. The finding was clear-cut. Only 18 percent of subjects in the experiment
were willing to make the cold calculation that saving the lives of several
people at the cost of one life was fair when the intended victim shared their
native language. But the percent willing
to sacrifice the person more than doubled when it was revealed they spoke a foreign
language. The experiment’s results
remained the same whether the subjects spoke Korean, Hebrew, Japanese, English
or Spanish.
I want to do two things in this piece while I have you
here. One, I want to convince you that
achieving empathy is harder than you may think when the person hurting is not
from your tribe. I hope I have done
that. Two, I want to show you there are
ways to achieve empathy and these involve tasks for which the historian is
particularly well-suited.
Let’s return for a moment to the Korean War. From the
perspective of 2015 Americans’ inhumanity seems appalling. Time gives us an
advantage contemporaries lacked. It
gives us distance. In historiography courses in college this distance is often
described as a disadvantage. For it’s
always hard to imagine what life was like for people living in a different time
and place. But what appears on the
surface to be a disadvantage in understanding a situation that happened in the
past is often actually an advantage.
It’s one the historian exploits regularly. It’s the reason historians
generally find it easy to spot inhumanity:
We aren’t wearing the blinders that block the vision of people caught up
in events. We ourselves and our children
aren’t at risk. As in this case, the Korean War happened to other people, not
to us.
Distance is not alone sufficient, however. To truly understand what the Korean people
were going through we need to get inside their heads. This is also something historians do as a
matter of course. We mind read. We plunge into the archives and read
everything we can get our hands on:
diaries, oral histories, any and everything that will help us see life
as the people we are writing about saw it.
In effect, one of our chief tasks is to be empathetic.
But there’s still one obstacle we need to overcome. Somehow we have to communicate our knowledge
to our readers. Here we fall back on an
ancient art: storytelling. It is this,
more than anything else that gives us the ability to help readers achieve a
powerful feeling of empathy for people who lived at a different time and
place. Stories hold the readers’
attention, for one. For another, they
feed our readers’ strong human urge to find meaningful patterns in human
behavior. As scientists
have now demonstrated in experiments on split brain patients, the human
brain is a natural pattern finder. It
wants 1 + 1 = 2. Mysterious may be the
will of God, but here on earth we expect human behavior to be explicable.
Stories connect us to
people in a way nothing else can. It’s
the reason politicians tell stories. Years
ago, the distinguished Harvard social scientist Howard Gardner wanted to
discover what highly successful leaders have in common. After reviewing the
lives of eleven luminaries, from Margaret Thatcher to Martin Luther King Jr.,
Gardner concluded that their success depended to a great deal on their ability
to communicate a compelling story, “narratives that help individuals think
about and feel who they are, where they come from, and where they are headed.”
These stories, he found, “constitute the single most powerful weapon in the
leader’s literary arsenal.”
When people are reduced to
numbers—as the civilian victims of bombing during the Korean War were—we don’t
feel their pain. We don’t automatically put ourselves in their shoes, which is
by definition what you do when you are feeling empathic. We have the bomber
pilot’s problem. We don’t feel anything for the victims. But historians can
help. Storytelling is in our toolkit.
All we have to do is use it.
Storytelling happens to be
in every human’s toolkit. We are all
born storytellers and attentive story listeners. Biology may incline us to turn a cold eye on
the suffering of people we can’t see in person and don’t know, but stories can
liberate us. We aren’t fated to respond
favorably to a demagogue’s howling.
Stories change us. It may not be
natural for us to feel empathy for people living thousands of miles away, but
we can.